Evidence That You Should Probably Be Taking Practice “Micro-Breaks,” if You Want to Maximize Learning.

Subscribe to the weekly “audio edition” via iTunes

I took piano lessons for many of my childhood years. And though there was never any question that violin was my main instrument, there were quite a few things about the piano that I really liked. 

I mean, not having to worry about intonation was freaking awesome. And being able to play more than 2 or 3 notes at a time? And really loudly at that? That was also pretty cool.

But as satisfying as it was to play a single note or chord, and have it sound pretty darn good despite my very mediocre piano skills, the challenge, of course, was stringing multiple good-sounding notes together.

Because, I mean, sooooo many notes! Two hands! Doing completely different things! Ack!

Of course, on some level, I suppose it’s all kind of the same, whether we’re talking about piano, violin, clarinet, percussion, or voice. Because no matter the instrument, making music is not about making one note sound good, but about connecting notes in a meaningful and compelling way.

Taking single, discrete, motor actions (a single note), and linking them together into a longer and more complex sequence of movements (a phrase). Which is a process known as “binding” in the neuroscience literature. And apparently, how the brain does this has been a bit of a mystery – though a recent study provides some pretty intriguing clues.

So how does the brain actually string together single actions into sequences? And why should we care? Like, does this have any practical implications for how we should structure our daily practice?

A simple motor sequence

A team of researchers (Buch et al., 2021) recruited 33 right-handed individuals to participate in a simple learning study.

Each of the participants was asked to sit in front of a computer screen and type out a particular sequence of numbers – 41324, for what it’s worth – on a number pad with their left hand, as quickly and accurately as possible.

They were given 36 10-second blocks of time to practice this number sequence, with a 10-second break between each practice block.

In other words, when the practice block began, the numbers 41324 would appear on the screen, and they’d practice typing out 41324 as quickly and accurately as possible until 10 seconds had passed, at which point the numbers on the screen would be replaced with x’s, and they’d sit there and look at the x’s, until the numbers reappeared and the next 10-second practice block began.

While engaged in this practice activity, the participants were all hooked up to an MEG (magnetoencephalography) device, which is a type of brain imaging technology that detects and records neuromagnetic activity within the brain on a millisecond-to-millisecond basis, allowing researchers to map this activity to specific parts of the brain (here’s a short video on how MEG works , if you’re looking for a way to procrastinate on practicing 😅).

And what were researchers looking for?

“Neural replay”?

Well, one of the theories that has been proposed to explain this binding process, is that of “neural replay.” Where during pauses in our practice, the brain basically reactivates the same neural circuits that are activated when physically performing the skill – only much faster. Kind of as if our brain were fast forwarding through the sequence of motor movements a bunch of times to reinforce what it just did.

So the researchers were curious to see if they would find evidence of this sort of neural activity during the 10-second practice breaks.

And did they?

Learning occurred when?!

Well, they found a couple things.

First off, typing out 41324 is not a super difficult task. And most participants got the hang of the task pretty quickly, with about 95% of their learning gains taking place within the first 11 or so practice blocks.

And when researchers took a closer look at the rate of improvement during this early learning period, they found that most of the gains in performance occurred not during the practice blocks when participants were actively typing out the numbers, but between the practice blocks, when they were resting.

I know – that’s a little surprising, right? Because this suggests that the break or periods of “waking rest” between practice blocks were as integral a part of learning as the practice itself.

But as one of the NIH researchers involved in the study noted1 – “It seems to be that most, if not all, of early learning occurs over these rest periods and virtually nothing is detected during the practice intervals.”

And why might that be?

Neural replay seems to be a thing

Well, this goes back to that theory about neural replay. 

And when the researchers looked for evidence of this in the MEG data, they found that indeed, during the 10-second rest breaks between practice blocks, the brain appeared to reactivate or “replay” the same neural circuits associated with performing the task physically – just 20 times faster.

A speed which suggests that this was all happening in the background, and isn’t related to imagery or mental practice, which generally isn’t time-compressed in this way, and takes about the same amount of time as performing the skill itself.

So what are we to do with all of this?

Takeaways

Well, when it comes to the consolidation of motor skills (i.e. getting skills into “muscle memory”), previous research has established that getting good sleep is an important factor in this process (btw, here’s a great video of violist Molly Gebrian summarizing some of the research on sleep's role in learning ).

But today’s study suggests that integrating “waking rest” into our practice could be a really important factor to consider as well.

There isn’t enough research out there yet to know exactly what the optimal formula for rest and practice might be for maximimal learning, so it doesn’t mean that we must always integrate a 10-second pause into our practice after every 10 seconds of practice. Because that would probably be pretty annoying.

But I do think it’d be worth experimenting with tiny micro-breaks throughout a practice session, between groups of practice repetitions.

Like, instead of hammering away at a difficult passage over and over and over until you’re satisfied with it, maybe…

  • Allow yourself just a few repetitions or a few minutes worth of repetitions to see if you can get the problem passage to sound just right. 
  • And then, whether you’ve solved the problem or not, take a nice, deep, meditative, one-breath micro-break (no sneaking a glance at your phone notifications!).
  • And then test out the passage again to see if the improvements stuck, or try it a few more times to see if you can get closer to the solution.
  • And then take another micro-break, and so on, until you’ve reached the end of your allotted practice time or finished your to-do list for that session.

 
You’ll undoubtedly have to experiment with the spacing and duration of these microbreaks in your practice so that it feels helpful, and not a frustrating interruption to your practice flow. And there’s a big difference between these within-session micro-breaks, and those longer practice breaks that you take between practice sessions, where you get sucked into a YouTube rabbit hole, only to emerge hours later, no longer in the right headspace to get anything productive done…

But whatever sort of microbreak schedule you end up settling on, what I like about this particular practice hack, is that regardless of the potential learning benefits, the idea of a microbreak is probably a good one to explore anyway, from a purely physical perspective. Because microbreaks are a nice way to release muscle tension and avoid that tendency to get progressively more tense and wound up as our practice session progresses.

So at first glance, reducing the number of repetitions you engage in during practice might seem like a counterproductive learning strategy, but if integrating a few short pauses into your practice sessions every few minutes could accelerate learning and also help to prevent pain and injury, it certainly seems like a hack worth trying!


References

Buch, E. R., Claudino, L., Quentin, R., Bönstrup, M., & Cohen, L. G. (2021). Consolidation of human skill linked to waking hippocampo-neocortical replay. Cell Reports, 35(10), 109193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109193

Footnotes

  1. Why Scientists Think This Hack Is Crucial for Lifelong Learning

Ack! After Countless Hours of Practice...
Why Are Performances Still So Hit or Miss?

It’s not a talent issue. And that rush of adrenaline and emotional roller coaster you experience before performances is totally normal too.

Performing at the upper ranges of your ability under pressure is a unique skill – one that requires specific mental skills, and perhaps a few other tweaks in your approach to practicing too. Elite athletes have been learning these techniques for decades; if nerves and self-doubt have been recurring obstacles in your performances, I’d like to help you do the same.

Click below to learn more about Beyond Practicing – a home-study course where you’ll explore the 6 skills that are characteristic of top performers. And learn how you can develop these into strengths of your own. And begin to see tangible improvements in your playing that transfer to the stage.

NOTE: Just FYI, the annual 2-for-1 event begins this Friday (Dec. 10)! So if you’re thinking about signing up for the course, I’d actually suggest holding off for a few more days! What?! Why?

‘Cause if you sign up anytime Friday through Sunday (Dec. 10-12), I’ll include a second bonus account at no additional cost that you can gift to a friend, colleague, family member, student, teacher, or practice buddy of your choice. 😁

Click below to learn more about the course and the holiday 2-for-1 offer.

Comments

8 Responses

  1. Thanks for this article. I read the science article and wondered how long of a break would suffice, so this adds to that. I’ll have to try this method.

  2. I’ve done this for decades, but without understanding why it seems to work. I’ve often found that I could stop thinking about a pattern and just start hearing it (without detailed thought) more easily after a brief break and noticed my students seemed to pick up new things quicker using the idea as well. Since it’s impossible to play together in Zoom lessons, they can be used for this technique as well. I may play a pattern several times in a row, say “your turn” have them do the same, then “my turn”, etc. I also cycle back and forth between two or three things I’m trying to learn and this seems to work as well, so I’d be interested if there’s any research on that.

  3. I posted a link to your above article on Piano World Forum to share with others.

    This idea could prove really useful, thanks Noa.

  4. Really interesting article Noa, thank you!
    This sounds like an effective strategy when first learning a skill using blocked practice, before moving onto serial and random interleaved once the skill is becoming more automatic. Would that be right? Or is this considered a condensed form of interleaving, and different to blocked repetition?
    I’m also wondering what the participants were thinking about in the wakeful rest periods? The study says that the screen had a line of X’s that the participants were to focus on, but was this coupled with mental rehearsal (which I think would happen spontaneously anyway), or were they trying to think of nothing in between repetitions?
    This article made me think of one of your previous ones on post-KR delay: https://bulletproofmusician.com/for-more-perfect-practice-trylongerpauses/
    I try to get my students to consciously reflect between repetitions, and have at least a 5 second gap, as per the research. But I’m wondering if it always needs to be conscious reflection, thinking, and planning, or is it OK to stare off into space for 10 seconds to create the “spacing effect” (pun intended!)
    I might be looking at this too literally, but I think it would have a bit of impact on the quality of repetitions, so I’d be keen to hear what you thought. Thanks!

    1. Hi Mark,

      Great questions! I see this as being like micro-distributed or micro-spaced practice, which could be used within blocked practice, as well as within serial or random interleaved practice too.

      As far as what participants were thinking about during the 10-second pause, it’s not clear. They were simply instructed to “fixate” on the x’s on the screen. So there was probably a good bit of individual variation between what they were thinking about. Some might have done some mental imagery, others probably just blanked out or daydreamed a bit, it’s hard to know.

      The tricky thing to parse out here for me, is that in this study, participants did a few repetitions in a row (presumably mindless repetition), and then integrated a bit of spacing out/wakeful rest, before the next few repetitions. Typically, if someone is engaged in deliberate practice, there would be a pause for some reflection between single repetitions anyhow. So is there an additional benefit to adding an extra pause to space out every so often, above and beyond the normal reflective pause that would occur between each repetition anyhow? I don’t think that’s something that this study can help us answer. I think it probably wouldn’t hurt, but I’m not sure how much of an additional benefit there might be.

      Incidentally, I could see these micro-breaks working well as a way to take a little break when you’re doing a bunch of at-tempo repetitions in a row, ala Jason Sulliman’s podcast episode, where you wouldn’t have the same sort of reflective inter-repetition pauses as you would in normal deliberate practice.

      Anyhow, hope that helps to answer your questions!

  5. Awesome, thanks Noa, appreciate the reply!
    I have seen Gregg Goodhart (www.ggoodhart.com) add in short 20-30 second breaks of “think about anything but the music” time when mental fatigue sets in, or to help clear working memory and aid in spaced repetition. I schedule in 30 second breaks every 5 minutes in my practice sessions just to give my brain a rest and have a schluk of water, so it all sounds similar. I like your idea of adding micro-breaks between at-tempo repetitions, I’ll give it a try!
    Thanks for all the work you do, and great info you provide, love it! 🙂

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Get the (Free) Practice Hacks Guide

Learn the #1 thing that top practicers do differently, plus 7 other strategies for practice that sticks.

Do you know your mental strengths and weaknesses?

If performances have been frustratingly inconsistent, try the 4-min Mental Skills Audit. It won't tell you what Harry Potter character you are, but it will point you in the direction of some new practice hacks that could help you level up.

Share480
Tweet
Email